As in Humans, Sleep Solidifies a Bird’s Memories

The research, published Wednesday by The Journal of Neuroscience, fills an important gap between human behavioral findings and animal experiments of how the brain changes after learning and sleep.

“We really wanted to behaviorally show that these types of sleep-dependent memory benefits are occurring in animals,” said Timothy Brawn, graduate student at the University of Chicago and lead author on the study. “What was remarkable was that the pattern here looks very similar to what we see in humans. There wasn't anything that was terribly different.”

In order to survive, animals must be able to learn from experience, and understanding the biology of this process remains an open scientific question. Previous research has demonstrated that sleep plays in important role in vocal learning in birds, and is also important for stabilizing memories in humans.

In 2008, Brawn and co-authors Kimberly Fenn, Howard Nusbaum, and Daniel Margoliash found that a night's sleep stabilized the skills of people learning to play a first-person shooter video game. That study built upon prior research from Fenn, Nusbaum and Margoliash in 2003 that found sleep helped college students retain perceptual learning of computer-generated speech.

But sleep-dependent consolidation had not been conclusively proven behaviorally in adult animals. So Brawn set out to replicate the findings of his human study in the starling, a bird known for its vocal production and listening skills.

Starlings were trained to discriminate between two five-second snippets of birdsong in a learning task called a go-nogo procedure. If they heard one song, the “go” stimulus, they would receive a food pellet after correctly poking their beak into a hole in their cage. If the other song, the “no-go” stimulus, was played, it signaled that the bird should not poke its beak in the hole, or else the lights in the cage were briefly shut off.

Groups of starlings were trained in the task at different times of day, then re-tested later to see how well they retained their learning. In all groups, performance on the task improved after the birds slept, relative to their performance before sleep. That result replicated the sleep-dependent enhancement pattern observed in human studies.

Another human learning pattern, called waking performance deterioration, was not conclusively replicated in the starlings. When humans are taught a task in the morning, their performance on some tasks worsen over the course of the day, only to rebound after a period of sleep.

Starlings also showed a slight decrease in their performance of the no-nogo task over the course of a day without sleep, but those changes were not statistically significant. Brawn theorized that the deterioration may be more pronounced in humans because, unlike caged starlings, the human task must compete with the rest of the day’s experiences.

“In human studies, we can't control what they're doing throughout the day,” Brawn said. “In starling experiments, they don't have any other interactions… I think there's a much reduced interference effect here.” Future experiments with starlings and humans can directly study the effects of interference on learning and how sleep may overcome those effects.

Regardless, the discovery of similar sleep-dependent consolidation of learning in starlings and humans opens up new possibilities for research into the mechanisms of learning and memory.

“The result suggests this is a very broad, general phenomenon that might be shared across a great many vertebrates,” said Margoliash, professor of organismal biology, psychology and neuroscience. “It was quite important to show that and it now opens the possibility for mechanistic and behavioral experiments in animals that are difficult to do in humans.”

Such exploration will continue a long collaboration between Margoliash and Nusbaum’s groups that offers the unique ability to simultaneously study the interaction between learning, memory and sleep in multiple species, with results from bird experiments informing human studies and vice versa.

“There are very few paradigms of research that have unfolded at the same time in parallel between using humans and animal subjects,” said Nusbaum, professor and chair of psychology. “These scientific advances suggest that we may be able to use birds to study the role of sleep in vocal learning and language development in humans.”

What's clear now from an abundance of data is that sleep is an important stabilizer of learning across species, suggesting a long evolutionary history of this particular function. Brawn said he was convinced enough of the phenomenon to apply it to his own study habits.

“If I have an afternoon test, I want to make sure I've done a lot of studying the day before rather than just doing it the morning of the test,” Brawn said.

The article, “Sleep-Dependent Consolidation of Auditory Discrimination in Adult Starlings,” will be appear in the January 13th issue of The Journal of Neuroscience. The research was funded in part by grants from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders and the National Institute of Mental Health.

***Video and starling photographs available on request. Photo credit: Daniel Baleckaitis.***

Media Contact

Rob Mitchum Newswise Science News

More Information:

http://www.uchospitals.edu

All latest news from the category: Life Sciences and Chemistry

Articles and reports from the Life Sciences and chemistry area deal with applied and basic research into modern biology, chemistry and human medicine.

Valuable information can be found on a range of life sciences fields including bacteriology, biochemistry, bionics, bioinformatics, biophysics, biotechnology, genetics, geobotany, human biology, marine biology, microbiology, molecular biology, cellular biology, zoology, bioinorganic chemistry, microchemistry and environmental chemistry.

Back to home

Comments (0)

Write a comment

Newest articles

Bringing bio-inspired robots to life

Nebraska researcher Eric Markvicka gets NSF CAREER Award to pursue manufacture of novel materials for soft robotics and stretchable electronics. Engineers are increasingly eager to develop robots that mimic the…

Bella moths use poison to attract mates

Scientists are closer to finding out how. Pyrrolizidine alkaloids are as bitter and toxic as they are hard to pronounce. They’re produced by several different types of plants and are…

AI tool creates ‘synthetic’ images of cells

…for enhanced microscopy analysis. Observing individual cells through microscopes can reveal a range of important cell biological phenomena that frequently play a role in human diseases, but the process of…

Partners & Sponsors