Orientation of Desert Ants: Every Cue Counts

Cataglyphis noda ants approaching their nest entrance − a small hole in the ground of an experimental tube. Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology/Badeke<br>

Desert ants have adapted to a life in a barren environment which only provides very few landmarks for orientation. Apart from visual cues and odors the ants use the polarized sunlight as a compass and count their steps in order to return safely to their home after searching for food.

In experiments with ants of the genus Cataglyphis in their natural habitats in Tunisia and Turkey, behavioral scientists of the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology in Jena, Germany, have now discovered that ants can also use magnetic and vibrational landmarks in order to find their way back to their nest − a small hole in the desert ground.

In addition, carbon dioxide produced by their nestmates’ breathing also helps homing ants to pinpoint their nest entrance. Hence, the ants’ navigational skills prove enormously adaptable to their inhospitable environment.

Returning to the wrong nest can be lethal
Path integration is a fascinating mechanism that ants use for orientation. It combines counting the steps after leaving the nest with determining the direction by using polarized sunlight. This method, which helps the insects to return to their nest, is an important survival formula in the barren environments of deserts. However, the path integrator is error-prone. Therefore ants also use landmarks in order to find home quickly and unerringly: Visual as well as olfactory landmarks serve as important cues. For ants, it is a question of life and death to find the right nest because they may be killed or at least attacked by resident ants if they enter the wrong nest accidentally.

Vibration or magnetism

It is known from leaf-cutting ants that they use vibrational signals for communication. That ants − like birds − also sense the earth’s magnetic field, becomes more and more likely. Therefore, the researchers in Markus Knaden’s lab, behavioral scientists in the Department of Bill Hansson at the Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, wanted to find out whether desert ants − adapted to landscapes providing a minimum of cues − are able to use magnetism and vibrational signals in the absence of other landmarks. “We were very surprised that this is actually the case,” says PhD student Cornelia Buehlmann, who performed the experiments. Trained ants of the species Cataglyphis noda pinpointed their nest without any problem if a battery-powered vibrational device was buried next to the nest entrance so that the ants could localize their nest by using the vibrational landmark. To exclude electromagnetic effects of the device, experiments were performed using the vibrational device without contact to the ground. The result: The ants behaved like their untrained conspecifics. They wandered around aimlessly. If two strong neodym magnets generating a magnetic field of about 21 millitesla (the earth’s magnetic field was, for comparison, only 0.041 millitesla) were placed above ground next to the nest, trained ants again found their home without any problems.

The experiments demonstrated the desert ants’ highly sensitive reaction to vibrational signals. However, it is unknown which sense is involved in the orientation using the artificial magnetic field around the nest. “This doesn’t mean that ants have a kind of sensory organ for the detection of magnetic fields. Their behavior could also be caused by abnormal neural electrical signals due to the strong magnetic field which were memorized by the ants,” says Knaden. Anyhow, neither vibrations nor strong magnetic fields are likely present in the natural vicinity of nest entrances. Therefore it is really astonishing that ants “remember” vibrations or a changing magnetic field as nest landmarks. Ants which have adapted to extremely inhospitable habitats seem to be remarkably flexible in terms of using all senses for navigation.

CO2 as an olfactory landmark

Carbon dioxide produced by the ants’ breathing is an olfactory signal that is constantly present at the nest entrances. That desert ants of the species Cataglyphis fortis use the CO2 plume in order to return to their home could now be shown by experiments performed in Tunisia. The ants ran upwind along the nest plume when the CO2 concentration was not too high and corresponded with the typical plume concentration in the nest vicinity. However, CO2 is released by all the nests and therefore all nests smell the same. Therefore the question is: How can ants recognize their own nest when all smell like home? “We could show in a series of experiments that ants primarily rely on path integration,” explains Cornelia Buehlmann. If ants were released in the close vicinity of their own nest by hand after they had walked to a feeding place, they avoided following the original nest plume of their own nest: The olfactory signal and the number of footsteps did not coincide. In order not to lose their lives in a foreign nest, ants trust path integration more than the chemical signal CO2. They only follow the nest plume when path integration tells them that they are close to home. [JWK/AO]

Original Publications:

Cornelia Buehlmann, Bill S. Hansson, Markus Knaden: Desert ants learn vibration and magnetic landmarks. PLoS ONE 7(3): e33117. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0033117

Cornelia Buehlmann, Bill S. Hansson, Markus Knaden: Path integration controls nest-plume following in desert ants. Current Biology, Online First, March 8, 2012; DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.029

Further Reading:

Steck, Kathrin: Just follow your nose: homing by olfactory cues in ants. Current Opinion in Neurobiology (2011), DOI:10.1016/j.conb.2011.10.011

Further Information:

Dr. Markus Knaden, +49 3641 57 1421, mknaden@ice.mpg.de
Picture Material:

Angela Overmeyer M.A., Tel. 03641 – 57 2110, overmeyer@ice.mpg.de
or Download via www.ice.mpg.de/ext/735.html

Media Contact

Dr. Jan-Wolfhard Kellmann Max-Planck-Institut

All latest news from the category: Life Sciences and Chemistry

Articles and reports from the Life Sciences and chemistry area deal with applied and basic research into modern biology, chemistry and human medicine.

Valuable information can be found on a range of life sciences fields including bacteriology, biochemistry, bionics, bioinformatics, biophysics, biotechnology, genetics, geobotany, human biology, marine biology, microbiology, molecular biology, cellular biology, zoology, bioinorganic chemistry, microchemistry and environmental chemistry.

Back to home

Comments (0)

Write a comment

Newest articles

Lighting up the future

New multidisciplinary research from the University of St Andrews could lead to more efficient televisions, computer screens and lighting. Researchers at the Organic Semiconductor Centre in the School of Physics and…

Researchers crack sugarcane’s complex genetic code

Sweet success: Scientists created a highly accurate reference genome for one of the most important modern crops and found a rare example of how genes confer disease resistance in plants….

Evolution of the most powerful ocean current on Earth

The Antarctic Circumpolar Current plays an important part in global overturning circulation, the exchange of heat and CO2 between the ocean and atmosphere, and the stability of Antarctica’s ice sheets….

Partners & Sponsors